Establishing a baseline: Documenting how Ecological Concepts are Represented in Children’s Books

native bee & honey bee illustrations for citizen science project

Illustration (c) B.G.Merkle, 2016

Background

Children’s books are important for education, because they have the potential to simultaneously inform and inspire, and they can do so in exemplary multidisciplinary ways. However, adults and children alike are at risk of being ecologically misinformed inadvertently by children’s books (Ganea et al 2014, Owens 2003).

The disconnect between children and nature (Louv 2008) may be compounded by children’s books (e.g., The Bernstein Bears; Peter Rabbit). “The tendency to reason about the biological world from an anthropocentric point of view can have negative consequences for children’s causal biological understanding.For example, research with older children has shown they have more difficulty accurately interpreting evolutionary change when those concepts are presented using anthropomorphic language” (Ganea et al 2014).

Yet, teachers working with Next Generation Science Standards (NGSS) are expected to use interdisciplinary materials such as children’s books to teach about ecological concepts (NGSS 2013).

Methods

I am conducting visual rhetorical (Foss 1994) and content analyses (Bauer 2000), of 100-200 children’s books in the UW/American Heritage Center’s Toppan Rare Books Collection, to investigate if/how five top ecological concepts identified by Lockwood et al (2016) and NGSS, are represented within these books.

I anticipate older books will be more accurate, while the principle inaccuracy will be anthropomorphized animals.

Results will be presented during ESA’s 2017 annual meeting, shared in an academic publication, and discussed in a teacher-librarian-ecologist workshop.

This project establishes an important baseline – educators and ecologists will have a more explicit understanding of the potential pitfalls inherent in using children’s literature to enhance ecological literacy.

20150323_Tiger at Billings zoo_poster_bw_rs

Illustration (c) B.G.Merkle, 2016

Objective 1

Document the extent to which ecological concepts are defined and taught according to NGSS vs. ecological representations conveyed by 20th-century children’s literature.

Objective 2

Build awareness among science educators by disseminating the results to ecologists and educators in accessible ways.

Book Evaluation/Data Collection Example

The tortoise and the hare fable features two organisms, which actually co-exist in ecosystems such as the Sonoran Desert, but blatantly misconstrues structure/function in the life histories of both animals. Children are led to believe the tortoise and the hare compete for emotive reasons: the hare is rash and prideful; the tortoise is humble and deliberate. This prevents readers from understanding the animals’ physiology, adaptations to their environments, and any interspecific competition that may occur between them in ecosystems where they co-exist.

Now, according to NGSS’s cross-cutting concepts (which are quickly becoming integral to k12 education), students in grade K-2 “…observe that the shape and stability of structures of natural and designed objects are related to their function(s)” (NGSS 2013).

Yet, if students had recently read a version of the tortoise and the hare during a lesson on manners, it is probable the students would try to make associations between the animals’ physiology and their attitudes and behaviors in the fable, rather than connecting with the animals’ ecology.

A content analysis coding frame will be developed (e.g., Quinn 2006) in which the top ecological concepts are core variables. For example, the tortoise and the hare would be coded as habitat (1); scale (0); ecosystem (1); community (1); organism (1); patterns (0); scale (0); systems (1); structure and function (1); stability and change (0). Depending on the version of the tale, all concepts present are inaccurately depicted due to: anthropomorphizing organisms (1); conflating wild and human habitat (1) and community dynamics (1); not depicting real species (1) or a real ecosystem (1); confounding structure-function relationships (1).

20161002_tortoise-and-hare_cover_v1a

Illustration (c) B.G.Merkle, 2016

References

  1. Bauer, M. (2000). Classical content analysis: A review. In M. Bauer & G. Gaskell (Eds.), Qualitative researching with text, image, and sound (pp. 131-151). London: Sage.
  2. Ganea, P. A., C. F.Canfield, K. Simons-Ghafari, T. Chou. (2014). Do cavies talk? The effect of anthropomorphic picture books on children’s knowledge about animals. Frontiers in Psychology 5 (172).
  3. Lockwood et al. (2016). Unpublished data/personal communications.
  4. Louv, R. (2008). Last child in the woods: Saving our children from nature-deficit disorder. Chapel Hill, NC: Algonquin Books of Chapel Hill.
  5. NGSS Lead States. (2013). Next Generation Science Standards: For States, By States. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
  6. Owens, C.V. (2003) Nonsense, Sense and Science: Misconceptions and Illustrated Trade Books. Journal of Children’s Literature 29 (1).
  7. Quinn, S. M. F. (2006). Examining the Culture of Fatherhood in American Children’s Literature: Presence, Interactions, and Nurturing Behaviors of Fathers in Caldecott Award Winning Picture Books (1938-2002). Fathering, 4 (1).
This project would not be possible without support from the following:
ahclogo

University of Wyoming Toppan Rare Books Collection

Maggie Bourque (UW Haub School of Environment & Natural Resources); Anne Marie Lane (UW Toppan Rare Books Collection); Jeffrey Lockwood (UW Dept. of Philosophy/Creative Writing Program); Jerod A. Merkle (UW Dept. of Zoology & Physiology)